Get Addiction Help (888) 804-0917

How We Get Addicted

Ê

Researchers have shed new light on dopamineÕs role in the brainÕs reward system, which could provide insight into impulse control problems associated with addiction and a number of psychiatric disorders.

Ê

A joint study by the University of Michigan and University of Washington found that, contrary to the prevailing conception, differences in individualsÕ styles of response to environmental cues can fundamentally influence chemical reward patterns in the brain.

Ê

Deeper understanding of these differences between individuals may lead to new preventive tools or treatments for compulsive behavior.

Ê

ÒWe were able to answer the longstanding question, ÔWhat role does dopamine play in reward learning?Õ,Ó says the studyÕs co-lead author Shelly B. Flagel, Ph.D., a research investigator at the U-M Medical SchoolÕs Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience Institute.

Ê

The findings were published online in the journal Nature ahead of print publication.

Ê

To understand what the research uncovered, picture the classic experiment in which a rat learns to associate a lever with a getting a food reward. (In this case, the rats didnÕt actually have to press the lever to get the food; the researchers were testing its power as a signal of the foodÕs appearance.)

Ê

What scientists hadnÕt yet figured out was the extent to which the dopamine released by the ratÕs brain was related to the leverÕs ability to accurately predict the appearance of food, or whether it made the lever desirable in its own right.

Ê

The answer, the researchers found, is that it depends on what kind of rat you are.

Ê

Think of it this way, Flagel says: Some people will see a sign for an ice cream shop and for them itÕs simply that, an indicator that ice cream is available nearby. But other people will have a stronger reaction to the sign Ð the tantalizing association between the sign and ice cream is so powerful, they can already taste the treat and often hurry to buy some.

Ê

The researchers studied rats that had been selectively bred for certain behavioral traits, including different proclivities for addictive drugs. Rats in the drug-prone group tended to focus their attention on the lever. The other group cared a lot more about the place where the food actually appeared.

Ê

Still, if the ratsÕ brains saw the lever merely as a signal that accurately predicted the arrival of the food, the dopamine reward for both groups should be the same.

Ê

However, if the dopamine reward was tied to the strength of the ratsÕ desire for lever itself, one would expect a different pattern for each of the two groups.

Ê

And thatÕs exactly what happened.

Ê

U-MÕs collaborators at the University of Washington used a technique called fast-scan cyclic voltammetry to measure the dopamine responses in the ratsÕ brains as they shifted over mere fractions of a second. Their analysis showed that the drug-prone rats got a jolt of happiness just from the lever, while the food-oriented rats did not.

Ê

And their desire for the lever continued, even when the food reward was removed.

Ê

The study additionally measured the ratsÕ ability to learn when dopamine was blocked and repeated the experiments with rats that had not been selectively bred.

Ê

Flagel, co-lead author Jeremy J. Clark, Ph.D., of UW, and their colleagues hope the animal model will help scientists figure out why some people are more strongly motivated by environmental cues and therefore at increased risk for compulsive behavior Ñ or, among addicts, relapse.

Ê

ÒWe have been interested in understanding how differences in temperament control our day-to-day behavior, how they determine the types of pathologies we express,Ó says Huda Akil, Ph.D., co-director of MBNI, a professor of neuroscience at U-M and co-senior author of the study. ÒThis study helps us understand how, in some situations, dopamine amplifies messages in the world around us, playing a role in controlling behaviors.Ó

Ê

Meanwhile, Paul E.M. Phillips, Ph.D., AkilÕs counterpart at UW, emphasized the collaboration, ÒCollectively the contributions of our groups amounted to something much more important than the sum of the components.Ó

Ê

Notes:

Ê

Funding: National Institutes of Health, Office of Naval Research

Ê

Citation: 10.1038/nature09588

Ê

Additional U-M Authors:

Terry E. Robinson, Ph.D.; Leah Mayo, B.S.; Alayna Czuj, B.S.; Sarah Clinton, Ph.D.

Ê

Additional UW Authors:

Ingo Willuhn, Ph.D.; Christina A. Akers, B.S., Paul E.M. Phillips, Ph.D.

Ê

Source:

Ian Demsky

University of Michigan Health System

Ê


Enhanced by Zemanta

Incoming search terms:

  • new rat findings
  • Tests to Catch the Makers of Dangerous Legal High Designer Drugs


Loading